Numerous media reports indicate that people can
perform superhuman feats of strength during great psychological stress.
Straitjackets were designed specifically to control patients in mental
hospitals who suddenly go berserk and are impossible to restrain. Even the
world of sport boasts isolated examples of superhuman athletic performances,
such as Bob Beamon’s long jump of 29ft(2 ½ inches – 8,90m) at the 1968 Olympic
Games – a jump that exceeded the previous world record by nearly 2ft(0.6m)!
World records are usually broken by inches or centimeters or, more often, mere
fractions of inches or centimeters. Beamon’s record stood unbroken till 1991!
Women experience similar, or even greater,
percentage increases in strength compared with men who participate in the same
training program, but women generally do not experience as much hypertrophy.
Similar findings have been reported in children.
This does not mean that muscle size is
unimportant in the ultimate strength potential of the muscle. Size is extremely
important, as revealed by the existing men’s and women’s world records for
competitive weight-lifting. As weight
classification increases(implying increased muscle mass), so does the record
for the total weight lifted. However, examples of superhuman strength and
studies on women and children indicate that the mechanisms associated with
strength gains are very complex and are not completely understood at this time.
Obviously, increased muscle size is important, but there is increasing evidence
that the neural control of the trained muscle is also altered, allowing a
greater force production from the muscle.
Neural control of strength gains
An important neural component explains at least
some of the strength gains that result from resistance training. Enoka has made
a convincing argument that strength gains can be achieved without structural
changes in muscle but not without neural adaptations. Thus, strength is not
solely a property of the muscle. Rather, it is a property of the motor system.
Motor unit recruitment, stimulation frequency, and other neural factors are
also quite important to strength gains. They may well explain most, if not
all, strength gains that occur in the absence of hypertrophy, as well as
episodic superhuman feats of strength.
Synchronization
and recruitment of additional motor units
Motor units are generally recruited
asynchronously; they are not all called on at the same instant. They are
controlled by a number of different neurons that can transmit either excitatory
or inhibitory impulses. Whether the muscle fibers contract or stay relaxed
depends on the summation of the many impulses received by the given motor unit
at any one time. The motor unit is activated and its muscle fibers contract only
when the incoming excitatory impulses exceed the inhibitory impulses and the
threshold is met.
Strength gains may result from changes in the
connections between motor neurons located in the spinal cord, allowing motor
units to act more synchronously, facilitating contraction, and increasing the
muscle’s ability to generate force. There is good evidence to support increased
motor unit synchronization with resistance training; but there is still
controversy as to whether synchronization of motor unit activation produces a
more forceful contraction. It is clear, however, that synchronization does
improve the rate of force development and the capability to exert steady
forces.
An alternate possibility is simply that more
motor units are recruited to perform the given task, independent of whether
these motor units act in unison. Such improvement in recruitment patterns could
result from an increase in neural drive to the alpha-motor neurons during
maximal contraction. This increase in neural drive could also increase the
frequency of discharge(rate coding) of the motor units. It is also possible
that the inhibitory impulses are reduced, allowing more motor untis to be
activated, or to be activated at a higher frequency.
Increased
rate coding of motor units
The increase in neural drive of alpha-motor
neurons could also increase the frequency of discharge, or rate coding, of
their motor units. Recall from previous threads that as the frequency of
stimulation of a given motor unit is increased, the muscle eventually reaches a
state of tetanus, producing the
absolute peak force or tension of the muscle fiber or motor unit. There is
limited evidence that rate coding is increased with resistance training. Rapid
movement or ballistic-type training appears to be particularly effective in
stimulating increases in rate coding.
Autogenic
inhibition
Inhibitory mechanisms in the neuromuscular
system, such as the Golgi tendon organs,
might be necessary to prevent the muscles from exerting more force than the
bones and connective tissues can tolerate. This control is referred to as autogenic inhibition. During superhuman
feats of strength, major damage often occurs to these structures, suggesting
that the protective inhibitory mechanisms are overridden.
When the tension on a muscle’s tendons and
internal connective tissue structures exceeds the threshold of the embedded Golgi tendon organs, motor neurons to
that muscle are inhibited; that is, autogenic inhibition occurs. Both the
reticular formation in the brain stem and the cerebral cortex function to
initiate and propagate inhibitory impulses.
Training can gradually reduce or counteract
these inhibitory impulses, allowing the muscle to reach greater levels of
strength. Thus, strength gains may be achieved by reduced neurological
inhibition. This theory is attractive because it can at least partially explain
superhuman feats of strength and strength gains in the absence of hypertrophy.
Other
neural factors
In addition to increasing motor unit
recruitment or decreasing neurological inhibition, other neural factors can
contribute to strength gains with resistance training. One of these is
referred to as coactivation of agonist and antagonist muscles(the agonist
muscles are the primary movers, and the antagonist muscles act to impede
agonists). If we use forearm flexor concentric contraction as an example,
the biceps is the primary agonist and the triceps is the antagonist. If both
were contracting with equal force development, no movement would occur. Thus,
to maximize the force generated by an agonist, it is necessary to minimize the
amount of coactivation. Reduction in coactivation could explain a portion of
strength gains attributed to neural factors, but its contribution likely would
be small.
Changes also have been noted in the morphology
of the neuromuscular junction, with
both increased and decreased activity levels that might be directly related to
the muscle’s force-producing capacity.
0 коментара:
Постави коментар